Co-authored by Dr. Melissa Swisher, Lecturer, Purdue University
We rely on virtual assistants for many mundane activities–getting directions, calling a place of business, performing simple mathematical operations, and answering a multitude of questions. Amazon’s Alexa™ goes a step further and helps with homework. Does that mean students asking Amazon’s Alexa™ for help with their homework are sidestepping the point of homework: Are they missing out on an opportunity to learn how to solve problems?
When a student asks Amazon’s Alexa™ (or Google Assistant™ or Siri™) for information, they get an answer to a specific question. For instance, a student might need to know who invented the operant chamber. When asked, “Alexa, who invented the operant chamber?” Alexa™ responds, “B. F. Skinner invented the operant chamber.” Then, the student can write Skinner’s name in the blank or use that information to solve another problem or question. The student will now know that Skinner invented the operant chamber. However, by asking Alexa, the student missed the opportunity to engage in a few steps that might be considered part of the problem-solving process. For example, before we could ask Alexa™, we would have to find another way to get to the answer. We might have had to determine which unit covered the operant chamber, looking back through the textbook or class notes about early operant conditioning to find the answer. Learning how to find an answer without asking someone or something might be just as important as getting the right answer to a question, despite basing students’ grades on simply providing the right answer. That is, we can only acquire new knowledge by asking someone (or something). But what if we want to know something that is still unknown?
Other problems may require several precurrent responses (Levingston, Neef, & Cihon, 2009; Polson & Parsons, 1994; Skinner, 1984) before reaching a solution. For some problems, we might use a talk-aloud procedure: “To get to West Lafayette, you take I-35; no, that’s in Texas. You start at I-57, then take…the exit in Champaign…that’s I-74. You follow that until the exit around 30–maybe 34. Yes, that’s right.” This interstate-highway intraverbal of 57-74-231 might be visual bidirectional naming if the speaker-as-listener imagines driving the route and seeing the signs as she gives directions (Miguel, 2018; see also Weisenburgh-Snyder, Malmquist, Robbins, & Lipshin, 2015 and Whimbey, Lockhead, & Narode, 2013). Teachers can guide learners through overt talk-aloud problem-solving strategies (see Kieta, Cihon, & Abdel-Jalil, 2018) that proficient learners can use covertly to efficiently select appropriate responses. In fact, Axe, Phelan, and Irwin (2019) analyzed 12 empirical studies that referenced Skinner and problem solving. Various authors taught children and young adults behavior chaining, self-prompting, visual imagining, recombining units, and sorting for math, spelling, play, social, and verbal behavior skills.
Image credits:
- Cover image provided courtesy of Julia M Cameron under Pexels License
- Image provided courtesy of PxHere under CC0 Public Domain
- Image provided courtesy of Yan Krukov under Pexels License
- Image provided courtesy of Mikhail Nilov under Pexels License